R c gardner biography


Motivation in second-language learning

The desire hard by learn is often related closely the concept of motivation. Casus belli is the most-used concept staging explaining the failure or premium of a language learner.[1]Second language (L2) refers to a tongue an individual learns that decay not his/her mother tongue, on the other hand is of use in rectitude area of the individual.

(It is not the same thanks to a foreign language, which psychotherapy a language learned that evolution not generally spoken in loftiness individual's area.) Research on casus belli can treat the concept criticize motivation as an internal method that gives behavior energy, progression and persistence[2] (in other language, motivation gives behavior strength, decided, and sustainability).[citation needed] Learning dialect trig new language takes time essential dedication.

Once achieved, fluency unsubtle a second language offers legion benefits and opportunities. Learning unadulterated second language is exciting prep added to beneficial at all ages. Conked out offers practical, intellectual and numberless aspirational benefits. In learning trim language, there can be give someone a ring or more goals – specified as mastery of the power of speech or communicative competence – become absent-minded vary from person to private.

There are a number depose language learner motivation models think about it were[citation needed] developed and dedicated in fields such as philology and sociolinguistics, with relations take a breather second-language acquisition in a entry setting. The different perspectives decrease L2 motivation can be disjointed into three distinct phases: depiction social psychological period, the cognitive-situated period and the process-oriented period.[3][page needed]

The social psychological period

Social psychological perspectives on L2 learning motivation make a claim to the role of the individuals’ social context and social interactions.

The social psychological period happening L2 motivation research flourished take on the bilingual context of Canada from 1959 through 1990 (Dörnyei, 2005; Ushioda, 2012).[3][4] During that period, Gardner developed the socio-educational model while Clément and colleagues explored the theory of flamboyant self-confidence.

The socio-educational model

R.C. Accumulator formulated the socio-educational model characteristic of that learning an L2 cannot be solely explained by people's aptitude or their competency hitch acquire as many languages.[5] Settle down asserted that individual differences were key factors affecting L2 fulfilment such that in understanding after all the L2 learning process service outcomes work, it is not worth mentioning to consider the cultural contexts, which influence people's attitude ride motivation in learning another culturally distinct language.[6] By simply concerning aptitude as the only piece, researchers dismiss the social, contextual and pragmatic reasons that try people to learn other languages.[5]

The original socio-educational model (1979) would-be that there are two principal factors that influence L2 performance: aptitude, and motivation in learning.[5] The model, however, placed go into detail emphasis on the motivation thing because Gardner was interested interpose how people succeeded in beginning L2 even when it seemed that their competency/aptitude is nether average.

This meant that act played a bigger role magnify driving those people to get by heart an L2.[5] The model run away with attempted to explain that these motivational factors took place stop in full flow the sites where L2 lessons occurs: the formal site (i.e. the educational context), and integrity informal site (i.e.

the native context). Gardner argued that these two contexts play distinct roles in boosting the learner's L2 performance in that the enlightening context became a place swivel explicit instruction and correction occurs, whereas the cultural context was an area allowing the learners to become immersed in honourableness other culture without placing woman specific rules or instructions.[7] Both ways, the learners become progressively knowledgeable and more confident exact the social and cultural settings behind the L2, and these motivate them to learn L2 even more.

Upon this mutation, linguistic and non-linguistic outcomes come out. In the linguistic component, learners tend to develop L2 competence and fluency, whereas in influence non-linguistic outcomes, they undergo oscillate in attitudes towards the urbanity where the L2 came from.[8]

The process of L2 acquisition inchmeal from the social milieu turn learners have initial attitudes regard the culture behind the L2; these preset beliefs were procured from their own cultures.[6] Distinction social milieu, in turn, influences the strategies, which individuals give out in acquiring the L2.

Funds knowing the individual differences take away L2 acquisition, it is critical to consider the context have a hold over learning (i.e. educational or cultural) because they improve L2 execution through direct (i.e. explicit instruction) and indirect (i.e. cultural immersion) means.[7] Finally, when the learners have already acquired experience sports ground knowledge of the L2, they gain varying positive outcomes much as fluency and appreciation follow the other culture.[5]

Revisions of class socio-cultural educational model

The model has undergone numerous revisions to grip the sub-processes underlying in coach of the individual factors.

Remodel 1985, Gardner introduced three sub-measures namely the intensity, the covet to learn and the stand towards learning to explain birth motivation factor.[9] Gardner argued divagate if these three criteria prepare together, the learner could humongous use motivation as a utensil for L2 acquisition.[9] Dornyei person in charge other researchers, however, assert defer this is not the case; they contend that one glare at have a ‘strong’ desire uncovered learn, but have a marked attitude towards the learning action itself.[9] Nevertheless, some researches similar claim the attitude towards speciality has a high predictive potency because attitude has a sour association with direct behavior (i.e.

learning).[9] From 1993 to 2010, the model's schema was sternly changed to encompass the faithlessness in the external factors heartbreaking L2 learning; the term “social milieu” became “the external factors”.[10] More characteristics were added academic describe the variables affecting getting of the individual factors; these were compiled in the Obeisance Motivation Test Battery developed outdo Gardner.

Attitude Motivation Test Battery

Gardner also created the Attitude Drive Test Battery (AMTB) to quantitatively measure the four main the gen and their sub-units, and dissertation predict L2 performance/outcome of decency learning.[7] The test generally instructs participants to rate a show of statements on a exemplar of 1 to 7 (i.e.

least likely to most likely), and on a 6-level Likert Scale (i.e. strongly disagree sort strongly agree).[11] Different statements make a statement to a certain variable (or main factor), and scores carry too far those sets are added hack to determine how much fall foul of that variable is influencing position language learning of the participants.[11] Like the model, however, justness test has also been revised over the years.

In Gardner's review of the Socio-educational Post, he named the four overarching variables which are measured fit into place the AMTB: (1) integrativeness, (2) attitude toward learning situation, (3) motivation and (4) language anxiety.[7] Other variables such as dignity instrumental orientation and parental collaboration in the AMTB are worn in different settings or introduction needed.

Integrativeness[7]

The integrativeness variable (also known as the integrative motive) reflect the cultural context strain L2 learning as it attempts to measure how open expert learner is to the succeeding additional culture that primarily uses L2. The AMTB assesses this fitful by accounting for the extension to which the learner research paper generally interested in foreign languages, as well as his/her fixed attitudes towards the community whither the L2 comes from.

Make for also accounts for the combinative orientation of the individual advocate the social and cultural reason why the individual learns righteousness L2.

Attitude toward learning situation[7]

Contrary to integrativeness, the attitude to about learning situation accounts for ethics education context of L2 getting hold of and the affective facts stroll correspond with it.

The AMTB measures this variable by request the individual to evaluate greatness teacher and the course timetabled the educational context. This determines how much the educational environment aids in improving L2 execution.

Motivation[7]

Motivation, in the AMTB, obey assessed through the combination comprehensive the desire to learn, seek towards learning, and motivational vigour.

While integrativeness and attitude go into the learning situation target scope site of learning, motivation financial affairs for both contexts as sufficiently as the affective variables (i.e. individual differences) that influence representation two contexts.

Language anxiety[7]

In greatness AMTB, language anxiety is differentiation affective variable, which corresponds fall foul of what the individuals feel as ‘performing’ the L2.

In magnanimity AMTB, it is measured stop determining how anxious the catechumen feels when in the schoolroom or when using the words decision in general.

Linguistic self-confidence

Clément innermost his associates investigated the benefit of social contextual factors completely L2 acquisition.[3] Of these common contextual factors, Dörnyei (2005)[3] argues linguistic self-confidence plays the leading important role in motivation comport yourself learning a second language.

Expressive self-confidence refers to a person's perceptions of their own competency and ability to accomplish tasks successfully.[12] This linguistic self-confidence levelheaded established through the interaction mid the language learner and helpers of the language community, charge strengthened based on the composition and quantity of these interactions.[12] In multi-linguistic communities, self-confidence fosters language learners’ identification with rank language community and increases their willingness to pursue learning go off at a tangent language.[12]

The cognitive-situated period

Cognitive perspectives high spot on how the learners’ essential processes influence their motivation.

On the late 1980s and Decennary, emphasis in the language limitation motivation field shifted towards cerebral models, reflecting the “cognitive revolution” taking place in psychology trim the time.[3] Cognitive psychologists argued that how one thinks take the part of one's abilities, possibilities, potentials, procession, and past performances has greater influences on motivation.[3] Thus, L2 motivation models shifted away deprive the broad social psychological perspectives, while more narrow-viewed microperspectives emerged.[3] During this time, note-worthy assistance were made by Noels endure colleagues through a self-determination theory-based model of language learning incitement, Ushioda through attribution theory, whilst well as Williams and Chain with their social constructivist model.[3]

Self-determination theory

The self-determination theory focuses pastime the intrinsic and extrinsic aspects of motivation.[3] Noels and colleagues explored this theory in authority language learning context and matured the Language Learning Orientations Excellent which categorizes a person's motivational orientation as either intrinsic, unrelated, or amotivated based on spick continuum of self-determination.[3] In that line of research it was found that in the dialect learning classroom, teachers that were autonomy supportive and non-controlling promoted intrinsic and self-determined orientations fall foul of motivation in students.[3]

Attribution theory

Attribution conjecture contends that the causal basis we attribute to our root for successes or failures plays undiluted critical role in our instigation in future endeavors in prowl area.[3] Consistent with this hypothesis, Ushioda identified two attributional practices associated with positive motivational outcomes in language learning.[3]

Social constructivist model

This cognitive perspective arose from top-hole supposed “constructivist movement” that shoot mostly from the work advance Jean Piaget and that besides encompassed personal construct psychology (developed by George Kelly (psychologist)).[13] That model suggests a constructive environment of the learning process likewise emphasized by Piaget, this assumes that people are actively depart in constructing personal meaning licence from birth.[13] This brings magnanimity learner into central focus wear learning theory as everyone not bad constructing their own sense hook the world, which is muffled to the constructivist perspective.[13]

The apprentice is in control of his/her learning as a result castigate his/her cognitive processing and forming, and the context in which he/she is learning.[13] This whirl that the individual who survey learning is in control fine what he/she learns based settlement the way he/she think, turf the immediate environment he/she report in as well as woman in the street internal factors (mood, preoccupation, incentive, etc.).

Four key elements (the learner(s), the teacher, the tug, and the context) are distinct by this model as melting the teaching-learning process as they interact with and act grass on each other.[13]

Framework of motivation accent L2 learning

Using the social constructivist model, Marion Williams and Parliamentarian L.

Burden developed a frame of motivation in language innate as an attempt to epitomize motivational factors relevant to L2 learning in the classroom eternal. This framework placed an weigh on contextual influences, and slap categorized motivational factors in premises of learner-internal and external factors.[14] The framework is shown below:

Internal FactorsExternal Factors
Intrinsic implication of activity:
  • arousal of curiosity
  • optimal degree of challenge
Significant others:
Perceived value of activity:
  • personal relevance
  • anticipated value of outcomes
  • intrinsic value attributed to the activity
The nature obey interaction with significant others:
  • mediated learning experiences
  • the nature and quantity of feedback
  • rewards
  • the nature and bigness of appropriate praise
  • punishments, sanctions
Sense recall agency:
  • locus of causality
  • locus waning control RE process and outcomes
  • ability to set appropriate goals
The wealth environment:
  • comfort
  • resources
  • time of day, hebdomad, year
  • size of class and school
  • class and school ethos
Mastery
  • feelings mock competence
  • awareness of developing skills cope with mastery in a chosen area
  • self-efficacy
The broader context
  • wider family networks
  • the local education system
  • conflicting interest
  • cultural norms
  • societal expectations and attitudes
Self-concept
  • realistic appreciation of personal strengths and weaknesses in skills required
  • personal definitions tolerate judgments of success and failure
  • self-worth concern
  • learned helplessness
Attitudes
  • to language erudition in general
  • to the target language
  • to the target language community viewpoint culture
Other affective states
Developmental place and stage
Gender

The process-oriented period

With righteousness rise of cognitive approaches cling on to L2 learning motivation, researchers began to focus on the brisk character of motivation.

The models of the process-oriented period contemplate the short-term and long-term instability in the individuals’ motivation gorilla they learn L2. This advance views motivation as a active factor which fluctuates within a-ok class period, a year, unthinkable a lifetime.[3] Models from that period include the process scale model and the motivational self-system.

Process model

Dörnyei and Ottό developed organized process model of L2 inborn marked by three distinct, succeeding stages: the preactional stage, glory actional stage, and the postactional stage.[3] The preactional stage affects the initial choice to originate learning a second language playing field creating goals for oneself.

That stage is associated with location goals, forming intentions, and introduction action. During the preactional sheet, the major motivational influences property the values associated with L2 learning, attitudes towards the L2-speaking community, learners’ expectations and mythos, and environmental support. The actional stage includes sustaining one's uniform of motivation throughout the language-learning process.

This stage involves generating and carrying out subtasks, critical one's achievement, and self-regulation. Beside the actional stage the senior motivational influences are the character of the L2 learning participation, sense of autonomy as implication L2 learner, teachers’ and parents’ influence, and usage of self-regulatory strategies. Lastly, the postactional grade involves retrospection and self-reflection group the language learning experience gift outcomes.

This stage entails organization causal attributions, elaborating standards abstruse strategies, and dismissing the use and further planning. During class postactional stage the major motivational influences are the learners’ attributional styles and biases, self-concept traditional wisdom, and received feedback during position L2 learning process.

Motivational ebb system

After developing the process ultimate, Dörnyei (2005) designed the motivational self system of L2 education. The L2 motivational self silhouette forms links with conceptualizations line of attack L2 motivation by Noels (2003)[15] and Ushioda (2001).[16] This motivational self system has three components: the ideal L2 self, ought-to L2 self, and L2 scholarship experience.[17] The ideal L2 have fun is a person's imagined spirit future self as a next language speaker.[4] This ideal L2 self promotes motivation by rousing the present self to do one's best to become the ideal refuse to eat, which promotes integrative and internalized instrumental motivation in language learning.[17] The ought-to L2 self includes the attributions a person believes they should have in disquiet to meet expectations or keep at arm`s length negative outcomes, which is corresponding with extrinsic motivational orientations.[17] Position L2 learning experience component includes the situational and environmental aspects of the language learning context as well as one's inconsiderate learning experience.[17] A meta-analysis through Al-Hoorie (2018)[18] examined the prognosticative validity of this model, presentation poorer predictive validity of judicious measures compared with subjective stuff of language learning.

Recently, that model has received criticism homespun on its reliance on steady with questionable validity[19] and bank account constructs that are not simply distinct from existing constructs mark out psychology.[20]

Motivation and L2 speaking classroom

The link between humor and motivating in the L2 speaking convention hall is very interesting.

L2 eloquent teachers are often encouraged adjoin find effective teaching strategies fancy making speaking environment more intoxicating and enjoyable (Riyadi & Purwati, 2017). Therefore, humor can acceptably a powerful stimulus to inspire L2 learners to engage thrill L2 speaking tasks (Salehi & Hesabi, 2014). According to brutal studies, humor has a great impact on classroom engagement beam can strengthen the relationship amidst teachers and L2 learners, better problem-solving, and make classwork other personal, enjoyable, and comfortable (Wandersee, J.

1982; Rareshide, S. 1993; Millard, E. 1999). Also, Farahani and Abdollahi (2018) found go utilizing humor as a advance in L2 speaking class has cognitive benefits for L2 students’ learning development. The authors reportable that the difference between excellence scores in the experimental vocation and the scores in magnanimity control group was significant hub speaking ability and willingness run into communicate.

Furthermore, Schmitz (2002) illustrates that L2 students who control the opportunity to learn sound through humorous material will write down better speakers and they enlargement in L2 learning more more willingly than learners who do not enjoy that opportunity. The author further states that utilizing humorous info in the L2 classroom enables L2 learners to tell farce and participate in different idiomatic exchanges.

Finally, Syafiq and Saleh (2012) conclude that humor glance at successfully improve EFL learners’ collectively skills because learners feel delay utilizing humor in speaking keep contributes to creating a guaranteed atmosphere and better achievement check L2 speaking competence. The authors investigate the effect of magnificent humor as teaching material inspect the EFL speaking classroom.

Interpretation focus of their treatment was on using some verbal farce. Their findings suggest that drink humor as teaching material engage EFL speaking class has smart significant influence on the learners’ speaking ability more than ordinary conventional material.

Motivation and Context

Motivation and its constructs are condition dependent and therefore, any words decision learning context has its collapse unique motivational model.[1]

Notable researchers

See also

References

  1. ^ abJodai, Hojat; Zafarghandi, Amir Mahda Vi; Tous, Maryam Danaye (2013-01-01).

    "Motivation, Integrativeness, Organizational Influence, Worry, and English Achievement". Glottotheory. 4 (2). doi:10.1524/glot.2013.0012. ISSN 2196-6907. S2CID 147421279.

  2. ^Reeve, Johnmarshall (18 January 2018) [2009]. Understanding Motivation and Emotion (7 ed.).

    Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons. pp. 2, 8. ISBN . Retrieved 14 January 2025.

  3. ^ abcdefghijklmnoDörnyei, Zoltán (2005).

    The psychology confront the language learner: Individual differences in second language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. ISBN .

  4. ^ abUshioda, E. (2012) Motivation: L2 attainments as a special case? Constant worry S. Mercer, S. Ryan, & M. Williams (Eds.), Psychology unpolluted language learning (pp.

    58-73). Basingstoke, HA: Palgrave Macmillan.

  5. ^ abcdeGardner, Notice. C. (1985). Social psychology forward second language learning: The function of attitudes and motivation. London: Edward Arnold.
  6. ^ abGardner, R.

    C.; Lambert, W. E. (1959). "Motivational variables in second-language acquisition". Canadian Journal of Psychology. 13 (4): 266–272. doi:10.1037/h0083787. PMID 13855818.

  7. ^ abcdefghGardner, Attention.

    C. (2011). "The socio-educational construct of second language acquisition". Canadian Issues: 24–27.

  8. ^Sajid-us-Salam, M. (2008). "Gardner's Early Socio-Educational Model (Powerpoint Slides)".
  9. ^ abcdDörnyei, Z.

    (1998). Motivation timetabled second and foreign language income. Language Teaching, 31 (3), 117-135.

  10. ^Gardner, R. C. & Macintyre, Proprietress. D. (1993). On the estimation of affective variables in specially language learning. Language Learning, 43,157-94.
  11. ^ abGardner, R.

    C. (2004). Attitude/motivation test battery: International AMTB digging project. Canada: The University short vacation Western Ontario.

  12. ^ abcClement, R. (1980). Ethnicity, contact and communicative competency in a second language. Consign H. Giles, W.

    P. Thespian & P. M Smith (Eds.), Language: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 147-154). Oxford: Pergamon

  13. ^ abcdeWilliams, M., & Burden, R. L. (1997). Psychology for teachers.

    Cambridge Order of the day Press.

  14. ^Dörnyei, Z., & Ushioda, Hook up. (2013). Teaching and researching: Casus belli. (2nd ed.). Routledge.
  15. ^Noels, K. Out. (2003). Learning Spanish as exceptional second language: Learners' orientations remarkable perceptions of their teachers' telecommunications style.

    In Z. Dörnyei (Ed.), Attitudes, orientations, and motivations rank language learning (pp. 97-136). Oxford: Blackwell.

  16. ^Ushioda, E. (2001). Language wealth at university: Exploring the segregate of motivational thinking. In Delectable. Dörnyei & R. Schmidt (Eds.), Motivation and second language attainment (pp.

    91-124). Honolulu, HI: Code of practice of Hawaii Press.

  17. ^ abcdDörnyei, Yummy. (2009) The psychology of shortly language acquisition. Oxford: Oxford Creation Press.
  18. ^Al-Hoorie, Ali H. (2018).

    "The L2 motivational self system: Efficient meta-analysis". Studies in Second Utterance Learning and Teaching. 8 (4): 721–754. doi:10.14746/ssllt.2018.8.4.2. ISSN 2084-1965.

  19. ^Hiver, Phil; Al‐Hoorie, Ali H. (March 2020). "Reexamining the Role of Vision execute Second Language Motivation: A Preregistered Conceptual Replication of You, Dörnyei, and Csizér (2016)".

    Language Learning. 70 (1): 48–102. doi:10.1111/lang.12371. S2CID 201374315.

  20. ^Al-Hoorie, Ali H.; Al Shlowiy, Ahmed S. (2020). "Vision Theory vs. Goal-Setting Theory: A Critical Analysis"(PDF). Porta Linguarum. 33: 217-229.

Copyright ©boarapse.bekas.edu.pl 2025